Understanding the Use of Force By and Against the Police. Series: NIJ Research in Brief Published: November 1996 15 pages 30,224 bytes U.S. Department of Justice Office of Justice Programs National Institute of Justice National Institute of Justice Research in Brief Jeremy Travis, Director November 1996 ============================================= Figures, charts, forms, and tables are not included in this ASCII plain-text file. To view this document in its entirety, download the Adobe Acrobat graphic file available from this web site or order a print copy from NCJRS at 800-851-3420. ============================================= Understanding the Use of Force By and Against the Police Issues and Findings Discussed in this Brief: An NIJ-sponsored study of 1,585 adult custody arrests in Phoenix, Arizona, the eighth largest city in the United States, to determine the use of force both by and against the police. Key issues: The research conducted by Rutgers University and Arizona State University was prompted by a perceived need on the part of the Phoenix Police Department and NIJ to gather information about arrest situations, tactics, policy, training, and practices. The study surveyed police officers over a 2-week period in June 1994 and interviewed 185 suspects. Key findings: Force was used infrequently by the police and even less frequently by suspects. Suspects interviewed reported levels of police force similar to those obtained from officer self- reports. Among the other findings: o Police used some physical force in about one of every five arrests. Suspects used some physical force in about one of every six arrests. o Phoenix police officers are required to restrain only felony or belligerent suspects. In 20 percent of all adult custody arrests studied, officers opted to use no restraints. o When force was used by the police or suspects, it was typically at the low end of the severity scale. o Weapons were used by the police in 2 percent of all arrests. The weapon most frequently used by the police was a flashlight (12 times in 1,585 arrests). o Of 41 factors examined, only 9 consistently contributed to the prediction of police use of force. o The single best predictor of police use of force was suspect use of force. o Two-thirds of the variation in the amount of force used by police remains unexplained. Target audience: Law enforcement policymakers, practitioners, and police trainers ============================================= Understanding the Use of Force By and Against the Police by Joel Garner, John Buchanan, Tom Schade, and John Hepburn The Phoenix Police Department, in conjunction with Rutgers University and Arizona State University, designed and implemented a study of the use of force by and against Phoenix police officers. Conducted during a 2-week period in June 1994, the research was prompted by the need to generate systematic information on the role of force in arrest tactics policy, training, and practice. This study, sponsored by the National Institute of Justice (NIJ), was concerned with determining the characteristics of arrest situations, suspects, and officers associated with the use of force, as well as the amount of force actually used. Research results showed that, during the study period, the Phoenix police employed some degree of physical force in 22 percent of adult custody arrests and that suspect use of physical force was even less frequent. Further, in situations where members of either group did employ force, the levels typically were low. Despite a policy that requires the routine use of restraints in arrests of felons and belligerent suspects, officers did not restrain suspects in one-fifth of all custody arrests involving adults. Finally, a number of factors--most notably, suspect use of force--were found to predict police use of force (see exhibit 1). This Research in Brief explains the methodology used to measure the use of force, discusses study results, and presents the implications of these findings for future policy and research. Study method The research was designed to address some of the more important limitations of prior research on use of force. First, measures were developed to capture the range of force used within the Department's standard force continuum. Measures of the simple dichotomy between force and no force (or excessive and reasonable force) neither capture the full range of police or suspect behavior nor recognize the great variety of force-related behavior in law enforcement. Second, rather than focusing exclusively on situations that involved severe uses of force such as firearm discharges, serious injuries, or deaths, data were collected from a representative sample of arrests to permit examination of the range of instances where some degree of force could be used. A third aim of the study was to determine the correlates of force. Although helpful, descriptive data on arrest situations or force levels do not provide, by themselves, an appropriate basis for developing arrest tactics training. Therefore, researchers sought to understand the specific circumstances and situations where more or less force is used to accomplish an arrest and to use that information to improve training and practice in arrest tactics. Obtaining the information. The primary source of data was a one-page (front and back) survey completed by Phoenix police officers after arrests were made during a 2-week period in June 1994 (see exhibit 2). This form was used to record specific behavioral aspects of the arrests and gather information on police mobilization, the nature of the offense, and officer and suspect characteristics. From these surveys, detailed information was compiled about the frequency of specific officer and arrestee behaviors along five dimensions: o Voice (officer/suspect spoke in a conversational tone, articulated threats, issued commands, shouted, or cursed). o Motion (mode of officer pursuit/suspect flight). o Restraints (mechanical devices used by police to control arrestee, e.g., handcuffs, body cuffs, hobble). o Weaponless tactics (officer/suspect grabbing, twisting, pushing, shoving, punching, kicking, biting, scratching, wrestling, scuffling, and use of control holds or pressure points). o Weapons (suspect possession or officer/suspect threatened use, or actual use of martial arts or a baton, flashlight, knife, blunt instrument, chemical agent, canine, handgun, rifle, or shotgun; includes use of a motor vehicle as a weapon). The detailed survey responses were used to construct three measures of force--physical force, continuum of force, and maximum force. These measures were designed to capture the full range of force used, including low levels of force not traditionally examined by police use-of-force research. Multivariate statistical models also were developed to evaluate the extent to which officer, suspect, and arrest situation characteristics consistently predicted the amount of force used. The reliability of the police survey was assessed by interviewing a sample of suspects booked in the Maricopa County Jail; 185 interviews were matched to officer surveys. Both the officer surveys and suspect interviews were voluntary and anonymous. Overview of survey results Of the 1,777 surveys obtained during the 2-week period covered by the study, 1,585 pertained to adults who were booked by Phoenix police officers at the Maricopa County Jail (noncustody and juvenile arrests were excluded). The Department's automated information system (PACE) recorded 1,826 arrests in which an adult suspect was booked. Thus, surveys were obtained for more than 85 percent of arrests involving detained adults. The surveys revealed that, in making these 1,585 adult custody arrests, officers: o Used threats or shouts less than 4 percent of the time. o Pursued a fleeing suspect 7 percent of the time. o Placed cuffs or restraints on 77 percent of the suspects. o Used a weaponless tactic (holding, hitting, etc.) in 17 percent of the arrests. o Threatened to, but did not, use a weapon 3.7 percent of the time. o Used a weapon in 2 percent of the arrests. Surveyed officers also reported that the weapon most frequently used by them against a suspect was a flashlight (12 arrests). They also noted that no restraints were used in 20 percent of the surveyed cases (3 percent of completed surveys did not indicate whether restraints were used). According to survey data, use of force was reported rarely by either police officers or suspects; when some form of force was used, it was typically at the low end of the study's measures. The three measures of force Physical force. "Physical force" was defined as officer or suspect use of any weaponless tactic (such as kicking or shoving) or the threatened or actual use of any weapon. In addition, police use of physical force included the application of severe restraints, including cuffing suspects while they were prone, hobbling (hog-tying a suspect's legs), leg cuffing, and body restraint (e.g., a straitjacket). The possibility that suspects would use restraints on police officers was excluded from consideration. Suspect possession of a weapon, even if not used or threatened, was included as an example of physical force by suspects. Officer possession of a weapon was not considered use of physical force by officers. In 349 of the 1,585 surveyed arrests (22 percent), the police reported using some form of physical force. In nearly four out of every five adult custody arrests, police officers used no physical force at all. The surveys indicated that suspects used physical force in 228 (14.4 percent) of the 1,585 incidents. In roughly five out of every six adult arrests, suspects reportedly used no physical force. Continuum of force. The study's second measure of force was based on two six-step rankings used by the Phoenix Police Department (see exhibit 3), which are similar to those used in many other jurisdictions to locate degrees of force in a progression. The rankings are independent; a "3" on the police scale (control and restraint) is not necessarily equivalent to a "3" on the suspect scale (passive resistance). The measurement of this "continuum of force" was intended not only to reflect the official policies of the Phoenix Police Department but also to incorporate the widely held notion that the force/no force dichotomy is inadequate to capture all important variations in the ways police handle encounters with the public. Exhibits 4 and 5 show the highest level of force used by police and suspects in terms of the force continuum. In 918 (57.9 percent) arrests, the highest level of force used by the police was some form of restraint; in another 350 (22.1 percent), no restraints were used. Chemical weapons were the highest level of force employed by the police in 2 (.01 percent) arrests; although not discharged, firearms were threatened or somehow used in 54 (3.4 percent) arrests. Other weapons and weaponless tactics were used in 261 (16.5 percent) arrests. In 977 (61.6 percent) arrests, suspects offered no resistance to officers. In another 196 (12.4 percent), the forms of resistance were either psychological or verbal. In 136 (almost 9 percent) arrests, suspects used or threatened to use a physical tactic or a weapon; in 11 (0.7 percent) of those arrests, the weapon was a firearm (see exhibit 5). Maximum force. A measure of maximum force was constructed by identifying the single most severe use of force employed by the police, based on a ranking of 80 different types of police statements, physical restraints, tactics, and weapon use. These rankings ranged on a scale of 0 to 100 (with 100 denoting maximum force). The severity of behaviors was weighed according to the judgment of 11 experienced and currently active Phoenix patrol officers, whose opinions were solicited for this purpose. In a similar exercise, a measure of the maximum amount of force used by suspects was developed (see exhibits 6 and 7). Suspects' views on use of force. In order to assess the reliability of the study's force measures, results of interviews with suspects from a sample of 185 adults taken into custody during the 2-week period were matched to corresponding police surveys. Interviewers from Arizona State University asked detainees about the force used by police in the course of their arrests, as well as the force that they themselves exhibited. The interviews resulted in findings that were comparable to those of the police surveys (see exhibit 8). Influences on use of force In addition to developing three measures of force, the study team compiled detailed information on how the police were mobilized, the nature of each offense, the location of each offense, the personal characteristics of officers, the personal characteristics of suspects, and any associations that could be made between police and suspect characteristics. Information about each arrest was collected on 41 specific items thought to influence the use of force. Analyses of these variables in relation to each measure of force identified 16 consistent nonpredictors, 16 inconsistent predictors, and 9 consistent predictors of force. Consistent nonpredictors. More than one-third (16) of the 41 factors examined did not predict any of the three measures of police or suspect use of force. These factors were: MOBILIZATION OF THE POLICE Custody status of arrestee Dispatch or on-view arrest Night time Weekends Weekend nights NATURE OF THE OFFENSE SITUATION Number of suspects at initial contact Number of suspects at arrest completion Victim and suspect friends or same family Bystander and suspect friends or same family NATURE OF THE ARREST LOCATION Inside a residence Location known for criminal activity PERSONAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE POLICE Number of arrests in past 30 days Years since last training PERSONAL CHARACTERISTICS OF SUSPECTS Known to have criminal record ASSOCIATED SUSPECT AND OFFICER CHARACTERISTICS Height Weight Given the size and representative nature of the sample of adult arrests from which various measures of potential predictors were obtained and the strength of the multivariate analyses employed, these findings of "no effect" cannot be attributed to either the research design or its implementation. Although generalization of these findings awaits replication, future discussions of police use-of-force policies and practices (and designs for additional research on police use of force) should consider this study's evidence that these 16 factors consistently failed to predict police or suspect use of force. Inconsistent predictors. Sixteen of the 41 factors considered in the study predicted only one or two of the three measures of force used by and against police. These factors retain their candidacy as predictors of force and warrant inclusion in subsequent research. Each predicted some measure of force but failed to meet this study's conservative standard of predicting all three measures. In the following list of inconsistent predictors, the minus (-) signs indicate a negative relationship. Thus, as a predictor (such as visibility) increases in value, the use of force decreases. The negative sign on the race variable is based on the finding that Hispanic suspects on average use less force against the police than non- Hispanic suspects. MOBILIZATION OF THE POLICE Patrol division (-) Early, middle, or late phase of shift Number of police at initial contact NATURE OF THE OFFENSE SITUATION Traffic offense Property offense Vice offense Domestic call Bystander's demeanor NATURE OF THE ARREST LOCATION Visibility (-) Inside a nonresidential building (-) Location known to be hazardous PERSONAL POLICE CHARACTERISTICS Length of service Past injury PERSONAL SUSPECT CHARACTERISTICS Drug impaired ASSOCIATED SUSPECT AND OFFICER CHARACTERISTICS Age Race (-) Because suspect use of force influences police use of force, this list of inconsistent predictors includes any factor that predicted one measure of suspect use of force. For instance, prior injury to an officer did not predict police use of force directly, but it did predict one of the three measures of suspect force. Similarly, the race of officers and suspects played no role in predicting police use of force, but because Hispanic suspects used less force than other suspects on one measure, race was listed as an inconsistent predictor of force. Age is one of the most consistent predictors of participation in and frequency of criminal behavior, and we were surprised that age was not a substantial and consistent predictor of the use of force. In fact, at least one of the various components of "age"--officer age, suspect age, and the interaction of the two--was found to be associated with all three measures of force employed in the study, but the direction of the effects changed with different measures and components. Consistent predictors. The analyses identified nine characteristics that consistently predicted police use of force: MOBILIZATION OF THE POLICE Use of contact and cover tactics Increased number of police NATURE OF THE OFFENSE Arrest for a violent offense NATURE OF THE LOCATION Presence of bystanders PERSONAL POLICE CHARACTERISTICS None PERSONAL SUSPECT CHARACTERISTICS Use of force Gang involvement Alcohol impairment Known to be resistive, assaultive, or to carry a weapon ASSOCIATED SUSPECT AND OFFICER CHARACTERISTICS Both male Some predictors affected police use of force directly; others affected it through their influence on suspect use of force. Some characteristics predicted both. Among the predictors of police use of force, suspect use of force had the largest impact on each of the three measures of police use of force. This remained true when controlling for the possibility that some suspect use of force could be a reaction to police use of force. This finding supports the perspective that underlies use-of-force policies and arrest tactics training in the Phoenix Police Department and in many other departments around the country: Police use force (and are authorized to do so) in response to suspects' levels of resistance. However, suspect use of force does not explain all or even a large proportion of the variation in the amount of force used by the police. This finding supports the perspective that response to suspect force, although significant, is not the only situation in which the police use force (see exhibit 1). Implications of study findings Policy implications. This research has specific implications for police policy, training, and practice in the use of force. First, it provides systematic evidence that the use of force in Phoenix is infrequent; when used, force was typically at the lower end of the measures of force. In addition, no evidence was found to show that force was applied unevenly or in discriminatory ways against racial minorities. The findings did raise some issues for further consideration. For instance, the single most frequent weapon used when arrests were made in Phoenix was the flashlight. At the time of this study (June 1994), the Phoenix Police Department's arrest tactics training program provided limited guidance regarding the use of a flashlight as a weapon. Officers are currently receiving enhanced training in the use of the flashlight as a weapon. The same rules that apply to the use of batons apply to flashlights. A second area of concern is that the widely promoted "contact-and-cover" tactic was consistently associated in this study with increased use of force. Contact and cover is a tactic whereby one of two officers makes contact with the suspect(s) or complaining citizen(s), while the second officer takes a position a short distance away to maintain a view of the entire situation and "cover" the contact officer. No assertion can be made on the basis of available evidence that the use of this tactic caused the police in Phoenix to use more force. However, the research design included controls for some of the characteristics of the arrest situation that might lead officers to use contact and cover--suspect use of force, violent offense, number of suspects and bystanders, low visibility--but these characteristics cannot explain the consistent association between use of contact and cover and all three measures of force. In addition, the contact-and-cover tactic is intended to provide officers with a tactical advantage should a physical confrontation occur. That advantage, some might assume, should reduce the amount of force used by police and suspects, but no evidence was found to support this assumption. A thorough examination of all aspects of the use of contact and cover, beginning with a more indepth review of the data collected in this study, is recommended. The third implication for policy stems from the finding that the gender of the suspect and the police officer directly affected the amount of force used by the police. More force was used by police when both officer and suspect were male than when both officer and suspect were female or their gender was unreported. However, male suspects were not found to use more force against the police than female suspects when all of the predictors were considered. For most officers and researchers, this is counterintuitive. Statistical controls for the height and weight of officers and suspects, as well as for the suspect's use of force, were included, and these controls (or other factors not included in the research) may account for the absence of an effect for suspect sex on suspect use of force. Since most arrests (1,059, or 67 percent) involved a male officer and a male suspect, we interpreted these findings to mean that arrests of female suspects by female officers involved less force than the typical arrest. A fourth area of concern is use of restraints and the possibility that Phoenix police officers are using too little force when they make custody arrests. In more than 20 percent of arrests, the police officers asserted that they had used no handcuffs or other restraints; in another 3 percent of the arrests, this item was not filled in on the officer survey. The available data do not provide a basis for determining the soundness of the current policy, which authorizes and encourages officers to restrain any custody arrestee but requires them to do so only with felons and belligerent suspects. The concern is based on the seemingly high frequency with which the discretion to not use restraints is exercised. The last issue raised by this research is the generic and imprecise quality of some of the 12 categories of suspect resistance and officer response that are central to the department's use- of-force policies. Behavioral indicators for each of these categories were developed, with difficulty, and it is suspected that officers may have similar problems determining whether, for instance, flight in a 2,000-pound automobile belongs in category 4, "defensive resistance," or category 5, "active aggression." Similarly, the policy groups together all weapons except chemical agents and firearms, which are placed in separate categories. We do not suggest the use of more than six categories or the kind of detailed ranking distinguishing weapon possession, threat, and use that was helpful for this research. However, the policy could be more clearly stated and the relative rankings based more explicitly on the relative severity of officer and suspect behaviors. Finally, as new weapons and tactics become available, the review of the continuum-of-force categories is inevitable. Research implications We used a representative sample of police behavior, developed a variety of measures of police and suspect use of force, and employed explicit models and appropriate multivariate statistical procedures to assess the strength of individual predictors of force. Previous research has not used these standards, but future research could benefit from using and improving on them. The sampling was representative but not systematic. We used one 2-week period in June 1994 and were unable to discern if seasonal variation played a role in the amount or distribution of force used. Other sampling schemes need to be developed and implemented that will provide a more formal statistical basis from which to make inferences about all arrests in a particular jurisdiction. Data collection was anonymous, and understanding the behavior of individual officers or suspects over time was thus precluded. Anonymity also complicated the matching of officer surveys and suspect interviews. Federal protections of research-subject confidentiality are strong, even for police officers. Future research could attempt to better integrate survey, interview, and official records of police and suspect behavior. The measures of force are improvements over simple dichotomies of the past, but, as ordinal or interval measures, they are simply illustrative. At best, they should be early prototypes of measurement models that reflect true scales or the full extent of harm caused by different forceful actions. The data collection instrument included many items thought to be important in the study of police use of force. The length of the form was a burden to participating police officers and reflected the fact that the researchers could not match officer responses to official records about the arrest, the suspect's prior record, or the officer's career. Improved data on weapon possession and use are essential, and the sequencing of officer and suspect behaviors should be a high priority in future research. The use of multivariate statistical models improved the rigor of this study, but the available methods were only a small subset of approaches to assessing causal influences. Most police professionals and researchers believe that community context is an important consideration in how much force is used. This research does not incorporate contextual models to account for such influences. Because this is only one study in one jurisdiction at one point in time, the results may not generalize to other jurisdictions and their relationships between citizens and police. There is no substitute for replication. Conclusion All prior assessments of police use of force that employed a systematic sample of police behavior as a foundation report that use of force is infrequent and that many of the factors commonly thought to influence it do not. Limitations in prior research left the validity and reliability of those findings uncertain. This research implemented a design to overcome some of these limitations, and the researchers reached the same conclusion: Using three alternative measures of force, the survey of adult custody arrests in Phoenix revealed that no force, or only low levels of force, was used in a large proportion of cases. In addition, the results did not support the notion that the race of officers or suspects directly or indirectly affects the amount of force used in adult custody arrests. The popular focus on racial factors in use of force seems to be unsupported by this study and other research evidence. Subsequent research must be attentive to the low base rate for use of force by police and the even lower base rate by suspects. On the other hand, official policy and training on the use of force do not indicate an awareness of the common absence of force. Recognition that force is rare and, when used, varies along a continuum has implications for law enforcement policy, training, and street behavior. ============================================= Joel Garner, Research Director, the Joint Centers for Justice Studies, was the principal investigator for this study. John Buchanan, Commander, and Richard Groeneveld, Lieutenant, Phoenix Police Department, served as project director and deputy project director, respectively. Co-principal investigators included Thomas Schade and John Hepburn of Arizona State University and Jeffrey Fagan of Rutgers University. ============================================= This Research in Brief is based on the study supported by award 92-IJ-CX-K028 from the National Institute of Justice, the contributions of the participating officers of the Phoenix Police Department, and the assistance of the Maricopa County Sheriff's Department. The complete report is available through the National Criminal Justice Reference Service on interlibrary loan or, for a fee, photocopy reproduction. Call 1-800-851-3420. Ask for NCJ 158614. Dr. Garner discussed his work with an audience of researchers and criminal justice professionals and practitioners. A 60-minute videotape, Understanding Use of Force, is also available for $19 ($24 in Canada and other foreign countries). Ask for NCJ 159739. ============================================= Points of view expressed in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position or policies of the Phoenix Police Department, the Maricopa County Sheriff's Department, or the U.S. Department of Justice. The National Institute of Justice is a component of the Office of Justice Programs, which also includes the Bureau of Justice Assistance, Bureau of Justice Statistics, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, and the Office for Victims of Crime. NCJ 158614